Fear nothing but phobia itself

17 January 2017 - 09:51 By SALLY SHUTTLEWORTH
subscribe Just R20 for the first month. Support independent journalism by subscribing to our digital news package.
Subscribe now
Image: Gallo Images/iStockphoto

The language of phobia is so common today that we scarcely give it a second thought.

Yet it was not until the end of the 19th century that medicine turned its attention to forms of irrational fear, following the initial medical diagnosis of agoraphobia (fear of open, public spaces) by German physician Carl Westphal in 1871.

Westphal had been puzzled why three of his patients, all professional men leading otherwise full lives, became struck with fear when having to cross an open city space. All were aware of the irrationality of their fears, but were powerless to overcome them.

The idea that sane and rational individuals could be afflicted with inexplicable fear was quickly taken up. When US psychologist G Stanley Hall published his Synthetic Genetic Study of Fear in the American Journal of Psychology in 1914 he identified 136 different forms of pathological fear.

These stretched from agoraphobia, claustrophobia and haptophobia (fear of touch), to amakaphobia (fear of carriages), pteronophobia (fear of feathers), and hypegiaphobia (fear of responsibility).

This created a vivid cultural and psychological map of the fears and anxieties of a society that had experienced the social changes of industrialisation and the decline of religion in the post-Darwinian era. Society was turning inwards, and to the sciences of the mind, for answers.

Hall's research on phobias stretches back to the 1890s, when he sent out hundreds of questionnaires for people to fill in about their fears. The answers make fascinating reading.

There is, for example, the English lady who claimed she had been "robbed of the joy of childhood by religious fears" and had decided instead to turn to the devil "who she found kinder".

A boy of 10 was more resourceful and decided to meet his fears head on. Hall wrote of him: "Decided to go to hell when he died; rubbed brimstone on him to get used to it, etc." A world of possibilities is opened up in that "etc". What else did the boy do to ensure he ended up in hell?

Hall argued, in Darwinian vein, that fears and phobias are largely the product of our evolutionary past, and come to us as inherited forms from our remote ancestry.

One phobia that attracted considerable attention was ailurophobia - the fear of cats. Medics themselves tapped into the public interest, writing in the pages of popular magazines. Neurologist Silas Weir Mitchell, for example, reworked a paper first published in the Transactions of the Association of American Physicians in 1905 for the Ladies Home Journal of 1906, giving it the far snappier title Cat Fear.

Like Hall, Mitchell also sent out questionnaires, exploring forms and potential causes of fear of cats. He was also interested in the seeming ability of some sufferers to be able to detect, without seeing it, when a cat is in a room.

Mitchell collected testimony of various practical experiments undertaken - cats tempted with cream into cupboards, and then unsuspecting sufferers lured into the room to see if they detected the alien presence. Initially he was sceptical: the hysterical girl who claimed she always knew when a cat was in the room was right only a third of the time. But he concluded that many of his cases could, indeed, detect hidden cats, even when they could neither see nor smell them.

In trying to account for the phenomenon he ruled out asthma, and evolutionary inherited fears (those terrified of cats are often perfectly comfortable on seeing lions).

As to the detection, he suggested that perhaps emanations from the cat "may affect the nervous system through the nasal membrane, although unrecognised as odours". Mitchell, nonetheless, remained baffled by "unreasonable terror of cats". He concluded that victims of cat fear record "how even strange cats seem to have an unusual desire to be near them, to jump on their laps or to follow them".

The internet appears to have intensified our fascination with cats. Where Mitchell and Hall sent out questionnaires to obtain data on fears, millions now write to experts to have their questions answered. According to one such site, Cat World, one of the most frequently asked questions is "Why do cats go to people who do not like them?".

The answers invariably invoke evolution: the frightened person is not a threat. But, like Mitchell, they still seem unable to answer the key question: why do only some people develop such terror in the first place?

  • Shuttleworth is Professor of English Literature, University of Oxford. This article originally appeared on The Conversation
subscribe Just R20 for the first month. Support independent journalism by subscribing to our digital news package.
Subscribe now