Does thinking you look fat affect how much money you earn?

Researchers Patricia Smith and Jay L. Zagorsky explore whether how we perceive our body weight affects our prospects in the job market and our earning potential at work

23 July 2018 - 11:15 By Patricia Smith and Jay L. Zagorsky
subscribe Just R20 for the first month. Support independent journalism by subscribing to our digital news package.
Subscribe now
The study found that women tend to over-perceive weight – they think they’re heavier than they are – while men tend to under-perceive theirs.
The study found that women tend to over-perceive weight – they think they’re heavier than they are – while men tend to under-perceive theirs.
Image: 123RF / Andreypopov

Two things people often think about are money and their appearance. Past research has shown that there is a correlation between the two: people subjectively considered attractive earn more.

And body weight plays a major role in attractiveness. A person’s body mass index (BMI) – which adjusts a person’s weight for their height – and their success in the workplace are linked. Put simply, thin people, especially women, are rewarded more than their larger colleagues. But those studies only considered how other people perceive you.

In new research, we looked at the flip side: does our own perception of our bodies, even when incorrect, make a difference? In other words, does thinking you look fat or skinny affect your wages?

Knowing if a worker’s own perception of his or her weight makes a difference – rather than only the employer’s – could help determine the best way to mitigate the impact of weight discrimination on earnings. In addition, a better understanding of gender differences in weight perception might help explain the persistent gender wage gap.

PRESSURE TO 'LOOK GOOD'

Americans spend billions of dollars each year on making minor changes to their appearance with makeup, hair dye and other cosmetics. They also spend billions trying to change our weight with diets,gym memberships and plastic surgery.

Trying to live up to the pervasive images of “perfect” models and movie heroes has a dark side: body-shaming, anxiety and depression, as well as unhealthy strategies for weight loss or muscle gain. For example, anorexia nervosa involves the extreme over-perception of weight and claims the lives of roughly 10% of its victims. 

Why is there both external and internal pressure to look “perfect”? One reason is that society rewards people who are thin and healthy looking. Researchers have shown that BMI is related to wages and income. Especially for women, there is a clear penalty at work for being overweight or obese. Some studies have also found an impact for men, though a less noticeable one.

DOES WEIGHT PERCEPTION MATTER?

While the research literature is clear that labour market success is partly based on how employers and customers perceive your body image, no one had explored the other side of question. Does a person’s own perception of body image matter to earnings and other indicators of success in the workplace?

In simple terms, does it change your wages if you think of yourself as overweight when you are not? Or if you think of yourself as skinny, when in reality you are not, does this misperception affect your ability to find and keep a job?

We were interested in answering these questions because it is often easier to fix your own view of yourself than to fix the entire world’s.

Our recently published study answered this question by tracking a large national random sample of the first wave of US millennials born in the early 1980s. We followed about 9,000 of them starting in 1997 when they were teenagers and ending 15 years later when the oldest was 31. Our research followed these respondents over a critical time period when bodies change from teenage shape into adult form and when people build their identities.

The survey asked respondents to report their actual weight and height. It also asked each to classify themselves each year as “very overweight,” “overweight,” “about the right weight,” “slightly underweight” or “very underweight.” This enabled us to compare each person’s clinically defined BMI category, such as being underweight, with his or her perception.

As in other research, women in our sample tend to over-perceive weight – they think they’re heavier than they are – while men tend to under-perceive theirs.

WHAT OTHER PEOPLE THINK MATTERS MORE

While self-perceived weight, especially when incorrect, can influence self-esteem, mental health and health behaviours, we found no relationship between the average person’s self-perception of weight and labour market outcomes like wages, weeks worked and the number of jobs.

In other words, it’s not what you think about your appearance that matters in the workplace, it’s just what other people think. Worrying if eating another cookie will make you look fat may harm your self-esteem, but thinking you’re overweight likely will not affect your earnings.

Because we find that women earn lower wages than men do even when accounting for weight perception differences, it appears the well-known gender pay gap is not due to differences in self-perceived weight.

While the continued gender penalty in the labour market is frustrating, our finding that misperceived weight does not harm workers is more heartening. Weight misperception is common, but thinking you’re heavier or lighter than you are doesn’t dampen earnings.

At the same time, it’s important to remember that although self-perceived weight doesn’t appear to affect wages, it still takes a toll on mental and physical health.

Passing over heavier workers to hire or promote less productive but thinner workers is inefficient and unfair. Our results indicate that expanding efforts to reduce discrimination on the basis of body weight in the workplace is important.

Since employers’ perception of weight is what matters in the labour market, policies to reduce the social stigmatisation of body weight, such as curbing body-shaming, make sense. Changing discrimination laws to include body type as a category would also help. 

We believe expanding such protections would make the labour market more efficient and fair.

• The author of this article, Patricia Smith, is professor of Economics at the University of Michigan. Co-author Jay L. Zagorsky is economist and research scientist at The Ohio State University.

• This article was originally published on The Conversation. Read the original article.


The Conversation
subscribe Just R20 for the first month. Support independent journalism by subscribing to our digital news package.
Subscribe now